Sally-Anne test

Jump to navigation Jump to search

WikiDoc Resources for Sally-Anne test

Articles

Most recent articles on Sally-Anne test

Most cited articles on Sally-Anne test

Review articles on Sally-Anne test

Articles on Sally-Anne test in N Eng J Med, Lancet, BMJ

Media

Powerpoint slides on Sally-Anne test

Images of Sally-Anne test

Photos of Sally-Anne test

Podcasts & MP3s on Sally-Anne test

Videos on Sally-Anne test

Evidence Based Medicine

Cochrane Collaboration on Sally-Anne test

Bandolier on Sally-Anne test

TRIP on Sally-Anne test

Clinical Trials

Ongoing Trials on Sally-Anne test at Clinical Trials.gov

Trial results on Sally-Anne test

Clinical Trials on Sally-Anne test at Google

Guidelines / Policies / Govt

US National Guidelines Clearinghouse on Sally-Anne test

NICE Guidance on Sally-Anne test

NHS PRODIGY Guidance

FDA on Sally-Anne test

CDC on Sally-Anne test

Books

Books on Sally-Anne test

News

Sally-Anne test in the news

Be alerted to news on Sally-Anne test

News trends on Sally-Anne test

Commentary

Blogs on Sally-Anne test

Definitions

Definitions of Sally-Anne test

Patient Resources / Community

Patient resources on Sally-Anne test

Discussion groups on Sally-Anne test

Patient Handouts on Sally-Anne test

Directions to Hospitals Treating Sally-Anne test

Risk calculators and risk factors for Sally-Anne test

Healthcare Provider Resources

Symptoms of Sally-Anne test

Causes & Risk Factors for Sally-Anne test

Diagnostic studies for Sally-Anne test

Treatment of Sally-Anne test

Continuing Medical Education (CME)

CME Programs on Sally-Anne test

International

Sally-Anne test en Espanol

Sally-Anne test en Francais

Business

Sally-Anne test in the Marketplace

Patents on Sally-Anne test

Experimental / Informatics

List of terms related to Sally-Anne test


The Sally-Anne test is a psychological test, used in developmental psychology to measure a person's social cognitive ability to attribute false beliefs to others (Wimmer & Perner, 1983). In 1988, Leslie and Frith repeated the experiment with human actors (rather than dolls) and found similar results.[citation needed]

Test description

The experimenter uses two dolls, "Sally" and "Anne". Sally has a basket; Anne has a box. Experimenters show their subjects (usually children) a simple skit, in which Sally puts a marble in her basket and then leaves the scene. While Sally is away and cannot watch, Anne takes the marble out of Sally's basket and puts it into her box. Sally then returns and the children are asked where they think she will look for her marble. Children are said to "pass" the test if they understand that Sally will most likely look inside her basket before realizing that her marble isn't there.

Normal children under the age of four, along with most autistic children (of all ages), will answer "Anne's box," seemingly unaware that Sally does not know her marble has been moved.

In the Baron-Cohen study of theory of mind in autism, 61 children were tested with "Sally" and "Anne".[1]

After introducing the dolls, the child's ability to name those dolls was tested (the Naming Question).

After this, Sally takes a marble and hides it in her basket. She then leaves the room and goes for a walk. When Sally is away, and therefore unknown to her, Anne takes the marble out of Sally's basket and puts it in her own box. Sally returns and the child is asked the key question: 'Where will Sally look for her marble?' (the Belief Question).

Two control questions were then asked—the Reality Question and the Memory Question.

The Reality question is "Where is the marble really?". This is to make sure the child had paid attention to the transfer of the marble from the basket to the box.

The Memory question is "Where was the marble in the beginning?". This is designed to make sure the child had not forgotten where Sally had left her marble.

For the children to succeed in this task they have pass the 'Belief Question'. The children have to show that Sally has her own beliefs which may not match up with how things really are. The correct response to the 'Belief Question' is to indicate Sally's basket. This will show that the child knows that Sally believes the marble is in her basket. The incorrect response is to point to Anne's box (Sally doesn't know that Anne hid her marble in the box).

Interpretation

Children who pass the test (presumably) understand that there are two different sets of beliefs:

  • their own beliefs, based on what they have personally seen, heard, remembered, imagined, reasoned, etc., and
  • the beliefs of others, based on what they have seen, heard, etc..

Children who pass this test are believed to have the following mental capacities:

  • to recognize that other people have perceptions/feelings/beliefs/thoughts/etc. different from their own;
  • to recognize that others may not know everything they themselves know, and vice versa;
  • to "mind-read" (or "mind-guess") other people's thoughts and feelings;
  • and to predict (or even interfere with) other people's third-party relationships.

Those children who fail the test are said by some psychologists to lack a "theory of mind." (In this context, "mind" refers to psychological processes such as perception, belief, thought, or memory.) However, failing the Sally-Anne test does not mean that an individual has no awareness of mental states: great apes and very young children, who typically fail the test, nonetheless show other sophisticated social behaviors (such as empathy).

Great apes and other monkeys

Since other great apes are not known to have a human-like theory of mind, it is assumed that it evolved after our ancestors diverged from other great apes. Suddendorf has suggested that this occurred with H. erectus (dating from 1.8 mya). Current research has failed, however, to disprove conclusively a theory of mind in other great apes and some new world monkeys outside the family of great apes, as for example the capuchin monkeys.[citation needed]

Criticism

Strictly speaking, the scenario presented in the test does not give sufficient information to determine Sally's expectations about the location of the marble. For example, it is not said whether Sally and Anne had previously discussed possible locations for the marble. Thus "I don't know" is, in a sense, the most correct answer.

A positive answer can be reached only by making assumptions about the unstated parts of the situation. There are many possible sets of assumptions that could be made, and the small number of likely answers means that the test cannot adequately distinguish between many of the possible assumptions. Furthermore, the categorisation of responses into "passes" and "failures" throws away most of that information. There are several ways to pass, and vastly more ways to fail.

These problems make the test of limited use as a diagnostic tool. The standard interpretation of the test identifies a particular stage in the most common pattern of development of social understanding, but it is misleading when applied to subjects who are not following the standard pattern. This is of particular concern with autistics, who are commonly diagnosed as lacking a theory of mind on the basis of "failing" the test.[citation needed]

References

  1. Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U (1985). "Does the autistic child have a 'theory of mind'?" (PDF). Cognition. 21 (1): 37–46. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. PMID 2934210. Retrieved 2008-02-16.
  • Suddendorf T. & Whiten, A. (2001). "Mental evolution and development: evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes and other animals." Psychological Bulletin, 629–650.

it:Test della falsa credenza nl:Sally en Anne

Template:WS